We couldn't find definitions for the word you were looking for. Or maybe the current language is not supported
00:00
A 'Hello Internet' fan who uses a rational rhetorical framework to sell people clothing alterations:
00:05
Tim the Toulmin Tailor.
00:07
Vivaldi's "Spring" rudely interrupted by a loud THUNK.
00:10
(one second of brain)
00:12
Way back in the dark days of episode one,
00:14
I put forward a limited set of ideas regarding the best way to debate with people on the internet.
00:18
Basically, I wanted to convey that taking the time to structure your thoughts made them easier to work with rationally.
00:25
Both for yourself and the people you're trying to convince.
00:27
I cited something that's familiar to many philosophers: a logical syllogism.
00:31
Which I still think can be a useful way of thinking about these things,
00:34
But it does have some drawback when you're talking about every day arguments.
00:38
Syllogisms are, essentially, deductive proofs which allow you to move from a set of assumptions to an inescapable conclusion.
00:44
Socrates is a man.
00:46
All men are mortal.
00:47
Therefore Socrates is mortal.
00:49
Badda Bing, Badda Boom.
00:50
The problem is, unless you're a mathematician or a logician,
00:53
deductive proofs aren't really all that useful in every day arguments.
00:57
I mean, as much as we'd like to think otherwise, people generally don't disagree with us because
01:01
they believe in something that's logically impossible which you can tear apart by using a syllogistic argument.
01:06
But, despite the drawbacks of a syllogistic approach, it probably still is a good idea to structure your assertions in some fashion,
01:13
rather than just machine gunning facts and feelings at someone and hoping that they put them together the way that you want them to.
01:19
Enter Steven Toulmin,
01:21
a British moral philosopher, who was kind of down on the very abstract approach that many academics took to the construction of arguments.
01:29
Toulmin thought that the deductive approach that they were using was kind of misguided.
01:33
That people generally don't build arguments to discover new ideas the way that mathematicians do,
01:38
but to provide rational justification for a claim, to convince other people that it's right.
01:43
He was more interested in rhetorical, inductive, arguments.
01:47
The sort that we make to each other every day to encourage certain behavior or beliefs,
01:51
Where we point out certain patterns and events and use them to assert what will probably happen in the future.
01:56
You shouldn't leave the leftovers out, you just know that Coco's going to get into them.
02:01
Note that these aren't as air-tight as deductive arguments.
02:04
You can't prove definitively that Coco IS going to get into the the leftovers the way that you can prove that two plus two equals four.
02:10
But, odds are, if you're trying to convince someone of something, you're going to have to use induction to do it.
02:14
To help people who are interested in building these sorts of cases effectively,
02:18
Toulmin posited a model of what he viewed to be the essential components of rational, persuasive, every-day argumentation,
02:25
using successful legal cases as a template.
02:27
He suggested that every rational argument could be dissected this way
02:31
to see which bits of it come together and where it was weak.
02:34
We start with the claim, the thing that we were trying to convince someone of.
02:38
Say, 'You shouldn't leave the leftovers out.'
02:40
Next, we gather facts that might be relevant to the claim, things like
02:44
data or statistics or even just observations that our audience finds credible.
02:49
Things like:
02:50
Coco ate that burrito right off the table last week,
02:52
Coco's vet says she needs to stop eating people food,
02:55
we want to save the leftovers for lunches,
02:57
Coco's getting fat.
02:58
So far, so every-other-argumentative-method-you've-ever-heard-of
03:01
But here's where Toulmin's approach gets interesting,
03:04
rather than just dumping data and/or conclusion in someone's laps and expecting them to figure it out,
03:09
we supply warrant
03:11
the mental connection which ties the facts to our claim.
03:16
Warrant is why Socrates is a man, all men are mortal, therefore Socrates is a turkey, doesn't make any sense.
03:23
The conclusion isn't linked closely enough with the things that are supposed to prove it.
03:27
In Toulmin's model, warrant is given its own explicit consideration,
03:31
so there's no ambiguity about what's happening in our heads between the facts and our claim.
03:36
If her past behavior is any indication, Coco will eat the leftovers if you leave them out, so you should put them away.
03:42
Notice that this is an inductive step,
03:44
highlighting some pattern in the facts, which suggests cause and effect,
03:49
and bridging that pattern to what will ultimately happen.
03:52
Warrant can benefit from backing,
03:54
facts which can't really prove the point by themselves but support the connection that's being made.
03:59
I mean, I wouldn't be able to resist mom's Turkey if it was just sitting out.
04:03
That fact doesn't have a lot to do with the Coco situation, as such,
04:07
but it does give us a little bit of a reason to believe that the warrant is justified.
04:11
Of course, in inductive arguments there are always some bizarre circumstances which would make the cause and effect relationship not work a hundred percent of the time.
04:19
For Toulmin's model it's important to reflect carefully on edge cases.
04:22
To explicitly note those boundaries where the argument does and doesn't work,
04:26
so we can make sure that this particular situation falls within them.
04:29
Like, maybe if Coco was asleep, or outside,
04:33
or the leftovers in question were something that she didn't like to eat.
04:35
Then it's pretty obvious that this whole thing falls apart.
04:38
But if she's awake, in the kitchen, staring at them drooling,
04:41
then it's probably valid.
04:43
As anyone who's studied persuasive rhetoric can tell you,
04:45
instead of waiting for our audience to come up with these counter-examples on their own, and think
04:49
"Ha-ha, you didn't consider this thoroughly."
04:52
We want to prepackage them in our initial assertion,
04:55
As if to say "No, look. I thought through every possible angle on this one, and this is where I ended up."
05:00
You might think that she's too full to eat anything else, and that might even be anatomically true,
05:05
but you know that she'll try anyways and end up throwing up all over the rug.
05:09
For me, this hedging process is where the Toulmin method really shines as a tool for rational thought.
05:14
By rolling this critical analysis into the argument itself,
05:18
you're forced to look at its weak spots, to find out where it doesn't work.
05:22
And then to either reinforce it or admit that it's limited.
05:26
Imagine if everyone had to do that before they clicked 'Share.'
05:29
That's not to say that every single thing that anyone asserts needs
05:33
every single part of this structure to be spelled out in order to be valid.
05:37
I mean, this whole deal with Coco and the leftovers could be adequately conveyed by:
05:41
DUDE! COCO!
05:44
But it can certainly help to have names for all the bits of a good rational argument,
05:48
both for making our own and for analyzing others,
05:50
Especially if we're looking to debate something a little bit more nuanced than whether or not you should put the turkey away when you're done.
05:56
Which leads me to one more thing that I wanted to mention.
05:59
If you're planning to get into some arguments, either on the internet or elsewhere,
06:03
during the holidays,
06:04
some computer scientists have done a fun bit of number crunching
06:08
to determine common traits of posts which ended up changing people's minds.
06:12
And here's the kicker:
06:13
They did it on Reddit.
06:14
In "Winning Arguments:Interaction Dynamics and Persuasion Strategies in Good Faith Online Discussions"
06:20
some researchers at Cornell dissected a huge number of posts from the "Change My View" subreddit.
06:25
An internet forum dedicated to level-headed, rational persuasive argumentation.
06:30
(pause for comedic effect) No, really.
06:31
The basic idea of the board is that someone will present some details about a position,
06:35
Then invite commenters to present their best cases against that position in an effort to change the original poster's mind.
06:41
If they admit that they're thinking about it in some new way,
06:44
they'll flag the post that pushed them over the edge with a delta symbol.
06:48
The researchers looked for patterns in the comments which resulted in deltas
06:52
and they found some interesting things.
06:54
For example: Posts tended to be more successful if they used personal pronouns like 'you' or 'me' or 'us'
07:00
possibly because they made the discussion more grounded and relevant for the people involved.
07:04
They also tended to mix up the language of the discussion a little bit,
07:07
using different words than the original poster used.
07:10
Commenters also tended to do better if they used concrete examples
07:14
or if they linked to external sources,
07:17
using references to make a point rather than asking someone to just take their word for something.
07:21
Successful posts didn't try to parrot anything back to the original poster, or lead with emotionally charged languge.
07:28
It seems that being angry or using someone's words against them isn't the best way to change their mind.
07:33
Question marks were also bad news.
07:35
Maybe rhetorical questions don't make the most convincing rhetoric.
07:38
And finally, commenters were much more likely to change someone's mind if they hedged their assertions,
07:43
using qualifiers like 'perhaps', or 'it's possible that',
07:47
defining regions where it wasn't entirely certain
07:50
if something was the case all the time.
07:52
That sounds familiar, where have I heard that before?
07:54
No, wait, I'm sorry. No rhetorical questions.
07:57
I said that a couple of minutes ago when I was talking about Toulmin's method.
08:00
Of course, these relationships are just observations of correlation, not necessarily causation.
08:05
And, the people on CMV are probably not representative of people in general.
08:09
There's no guarantee that doing things this way will get someone to come around to your point of view.
08:14
But, who knows?
08:15
Maybe investing a little bit of thought in how you structure your ideas
08:19
can make a difference in how people think about them.
08:21
Both other people and yourself.
08:23
Maybe even friend on Facebook.
08:26
Or people at a family dinner.
08:28
What do you think of the Toulmin method?
08:29
Please leave a comment below and let me know what you THUNK.
08:31
And, if you're interested in additional content, be sure to check out thunkshow.com
08:35
Thank you very much for watching.
08:37
Don't forget to blog blog subscribe blog share
08:39
And don't stop THUNKing.
All
The example sentences of SYLLOGISTIC in videos (1 in total of 2)
they personal pronounbelieve verb, non-3rd person singular presentin preposition or subordinating conjunctionsomething noun, singular or massthat wh-determiner's verb, 3rd person singular presentlogically adverbimpossible adjectivewhich wh-determineryou personal pronouncan modaltear verb, base formapart adverbby preposition or subordinating conjunctionusing verb, gerund or present participlea determinersyllogistic adjectiveargument noun, singular or mass.
Use "syllogistic" in a sentence | "syllogistic" example sentences
How to use "syllogistic" in a sentence?
Fallacious and misleading arguments are most easily detected if set out in correct syllogistic form.
-Immanuel Kant-